Skip to main content

Examining the impact of socioeconomic status, demographic characteristics, lifestyle and other risk factors on adults' cognitive functioning in developing countries: an analysis of five selected WHO SAGE Wave 1 Countries

Abstract

Background

Though extensive studies have been conducted on assessing the predictors of cognitive functioning among older adults in small community-based samples, very few studies have focused on understanding the impact of socioeconomic status (SES), demographic characteristics and other risk factors such as lifestyle and chronic diseases on the cognitive functioning among adults of all ages in a nationally representative population-based sample across low- and middle-income countries. This study, therefore, seeks to evaluate the impact of SES, demographic characteristics and risk factors on the cognitive functioning of adults across all ages in five selected developing countries.

Methods

Data from 12,430 observations obtained from the WHO Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health (SAGE) Wave 1; consisting of 2,486 observations each for China, Ghana, India, the Russian Federation, and South Africa, were used for the study. A meta-regression and a five-step hierarchical linear regression were used to analyze the data, with cognitive functioning as the dependent variable. Independent variables used in this study include SES; assessed by household income and education, demographic characteristics, other risk factors such as lifestyle, self-reported memory difficulty and chronic diseases.

Results

This study found that SES and lifestyle significantly predicted cognitive functioning in all the five selected countries as obtained by the pooled results of the meta-regression analysis. The hierarchical linear regression results also revealed that demographic characteristics such as age, type of residency, and self-reported memory difficulty significantly impact cognitive functioning in China, Ghana, Russia, and South Africa.

Conclusion

The findings in this study provide new insights for policymakers, caregivers, parents, and individuals, especially those in developing countries, to implement policies and actions targeted at improving SES and eliminating risk factors associated with cognitive decline, as these measures could help improve the cognitive functioning among their populations.

Introduction

The World Health Organization (WHO) has projected that by 2025, the adult population will increase to about 5.7 billion from the 4.7 billion in 2010 [1]. As the population increases at such a fast pace, non-communicable diseases such as diabetes, heart disease, and cerebrovascular disease leading to strokes, and the phenomenon of early onset of aging-related illnesses are becoming prevalent [2]. Additionally, risk factors associated with life course, with possible consequences not only for an adults’ current wellbeing but also for their health as they advance in age, are expected to increase further the pressure on adults and the healthcare systems [3, 4]. Owing to these, all countries are likely to face major challenges related to building effective and reliable health and social systems that can cater for this demographic shift, particularly those in developing countries.

For most adults, the maintenance of independence mostly requires physically and mentally draining tasks like  managing active daily living. Such daily activities can be inhibited by declining cognitive functioning, including basic cognitive abilities like memory, learning, reasoning, and knowledge [5]. Research has shown that people who have better cognitive functioning in the early stages of their lives have better health outcomes such as improved quality of life, lower risk of disabilities, and mortality [6,7,8,9] at an older age.

While cognitive functioning is crucial in measuring intrinsic capacity [10], evidence exists of the association between SES [11, 12] and cognitive functioning [13]. An improvement in SES, childhood health, nutrition, social support systems [14, 15], havebeen identified to help improve cognitive functioning as one grows. Furthermore, studies have found strong connections between socioeconomic factors, lifestyle, conditions of health, mental health, and cognitive abilities among adults [16]. Others have also revealed that socioeconomic factors together with age [16,17,18,19], sex [19], higher educational level [16, 19,20,21,22], economic status [17, 20, 22] and residency [17] remain very crucial indicators in determining better cognitive functionality. For instance, studies have shown a significant relationship between age and several different types of cognitive measures [23,24,25,26]. A study by Stephan et al. 2014 found a positive relationship between a younger subjective age and memory self-efficacy. This relationship has consequences for maintaining cognitive functioning with advancing age [27, 28]. Another study by Murman, 2015 observed that the aging of adults tends to accelerate aging-related diseases such as dementia by increasing the rate of neuronal dysfunction, neuronal loss, and cognitive decline [29], thereby impairing their everyday functional abilities. Marital status has also been found to impact adults' mental and physical health [30,31,32,33]. Studies by Sundström et al. 2016 and Feng et al. 2014 revealed that unmarried adults have a high propensity of suffering from cognitive impairments leading to dementia than married adults [34, 35]. Previous research has also observed that rural dwellers tend to have a significantly higher prevalence of cognitive limitations than their counterparts in urban residences [36, 37]. Research on the association between sex and cognitive functioning has produced mixed results. While studies have observed that  differences in sex is correlated with cognitive functioning [38,39,40,41], others have found comparable cognitive functioning in both sexes [42, 43]. Given that these factors have implications for an adults’ cognitive functioning, it is imperative to understand them in the context of developing countries in a nationally representative manner. Aside from these factors, lifestyle risk factors like tobacco use and alcohol consumption [44] have been established to negatively impact adults’ cognitive performance . The existence of these empirical pieces of evidence only gives credence to the fact that SES, lifestyle, demographics and chronic diseases have permeative consequences on health outcomes, including neurological conditions such as cognitive functioning across an adults’ life course.

Notwithstanding the plethora of literature, only a few studies have sought to understand the impact of SES, lifestyle, demographics and chronic disease on the overall effect on cognitive abilities, especially among adults across all ages. Most of these studies have largely focused on understanding the role these predictors play on cognitive functioning only among older adults in low- and middle countries. Others have also limited their studies to small community-based samples in developing countries; hence, the reliability and validity of the results cannot be generalized to a wider population due to the several differences in both country geographic and socioeconomic indicators. These limitations suggest the need to employ more representative population-based samples to explain better the phenomenon relating to the assessment of predictors of cognitive functioning among adults (both young adults and elderly) in low- and middle-income countries.

Thus, this study seeks to address these gaps by evaluating the impact of socioeconomic status, demographic characteristics and risk factors on the cognitive functioning of adults across all ages and to further assess their contributing role in cognitive functioning disparities in five selected low- and middle-income countries, using a nationally representative population-based sample from the WHO Global Ageing and Adult Health (SAGE) Wave 1 data [45, 46].

Methodology

Sample, sampling procedure, and data collection

The sample for this study was made up of adults aged 18 + in five (China, Ghana, India, Russian Federation, and South Africa) of the six Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health (SAGE) Wave 1 countries, excluding Mexico. Mexico was excluded due to substantially incomplete and missing data, especially demographic characteristics. A total of 12,430 observations were used for this study, consisting of 2,486 observations each for China, Ghana, India, the Russian Federation, and South Africa. SAGE Wave 1 is a cross-sectional study that provides baseline survey and biomarker data for nationally representative samples of adults' health and wellbeing from six low and middle-income countries (China, Ghana, India, Mexico, Russian Federation, and South Africa) [46, 47].

The data were collected via in-person structured interviews; paper and pencil interviews (PAPI) in Ghana, India, Russian Federation, and South Africa, and 50% computer-assisted personal interviews (CAPI), 50% PAPI in China [48]. The selection of samples for this study was done by implementing multistage cluster sampling strategies.

To preserve the confidentiality and anonymity of respondents, personal data identifying respondents were removed from the data across all the countries involved in the study.

Measures

Outcome variable

In line with the aim of this study, cognitive functioning was selected as the outcome variable. Cognitive functioning was evaluated using a cognitive battery test, consisting of forward digit span and backward digit span, immediate recall, verbal recall, and verbal fluency [46]. This was done by allowing respondents to repeatedly read forward and backward the lists of numbers in series. These readings were then scored, with respondents who can repeat these numbers without mistakes considered to have better recall and focus. Immediate and delayed verbal recalls were used to assess respondents' memory and learning ability . A 10-wordlist of animals was read out to respondents to listen carefully and  remember as many words as possible. This was done iteratively for three trials, with better recall scores indicating higher learning and memory ability. These scores were standardized and added up to evaluate the cognitive functioning of respondents, with higher standardized scores depicting better cognitive functioning [46].

Independent variables

The independent variable used to predict cognitive functioning in this study included socioeconomic status which was evaluated using the household income quantiles and the educational level of respondents, type of residence, sex, marital status, lifestyle, chronic diseases, and self-rated memory difficulty. Studies have found education and income to be among the key predictors of mental wellbeing among adults [49,50,51]. More significantly, these studies have consistently identified a strong positive relationship between higher education and better cognition [49, 50, 52, 53]. Thus, the inclusion of these indicators is essential in understanding adults’ cognitive health. In this study, household income was grouped into five income levels; lowest, low, moderate, higher, and highest income quantiles, while respondents' educational level was segregated into no formal education, less than primary school, the primary school completed, the secondary school completed, high school (or equivalent) completed, college/university/postgraduate degree completed. The secondary school completed and high school (or equivalent) completed were not merged since the five selected countries had different educational systems. This study categorized age into seven bands; 18–24, 25–34, 35–44, 45–54, 55–64, 65–74, and 75 + , type of residence; rural and urban, sex; male and female, and marital status; never married, currently married, cohabiting, separated/divorced, widowed. Lifestyle was assessed using alcohol and tobacco consumption, the number of fruits and vegetables consumed per day, while chronic diseases were evaluated using stroke, diabetes, chronic lung disease, depression, anxiety and hypertension. Lastly, respondents’ self-reported memory difficulty was rated as very bad, bad, moderate, good and very good.

Data analysis

The data analysis for this study included a reliability test for the outcome variable, descriptive analysis, a meta-regression and hierarchical linear regression using STATA SE version 15.0 (Stata Corp, college station, Tx) and Intellectus statistics (Intellectus Statistics [Online computer software], 2020). The descriptive analysis was performed on the nominal variables of the sample demographic by assessing their frequencies and percentages. Summary statistics were also computed using averages, skewness, median, and kurtosis [54]. The likely score of heterogeneity was evaluated via a meta-regression. Using the Higgins' I2 statistic, the homogeneity of cognitive functioning across the selected countries was assessed. An I2 score above 50% suggests a high heterogeneity [55]. The 95% Confidence Intervals (CI) of cognitive functioning was examined using the Dersimonian and Laird random-effect model. A graphical representation of the estimates is presented in a forest plot. The hierarchical linear regression was conducted to assess the level of contribution of the various variables in predicting cognitive functioning and also to determine which of the variables explains significantly more variance of the dependent variable.

Finally, to account for the complexity in the survey data, the survey datasets (svyset) command was run in Stata using the sampling weights, primary sampling units (PSU), and strata variables for the in-country samples and in the multi-country data set.

Results

Reliability test for items used to assess cognitive functioning

Results for the reliability test for the items used to evaluate cognitive functioning revealed that Cronbach alpha values were mostly within the moderate thresholds [56, 57] for almost all the selected countries (see supplementary material Table A1).

Sample demographic characteristics

Sample demographic characteristics consisting of frequencies and percentages for the variables used for this study are presented in Table 1. The most frequently observed residence category for China, Ghana, and India was Rural, while for Russia and South Africa was Urban. Regarding sex, the female category was the most frequently observed category for Ghana, Russia, and South Africa, while that for China and India was male. The majority of respondents for all the selected countries were between ages 55 to 64, while the majority were currently married at the time of the study for all countries.

Table 1 Frequency and percentage table for sample socio-demographic characteristics

The most frequently observed category for the level of education in China was respondents who had completed secondary school, Ghana and India those who had no formal education, Russia those who had completed high school (or equivalent), and those who completed primary school for South Africa.

Tobacco consumption was highest among respondents from China and India, while alcohol consumption was prevalent in China, Ghana, and Russia. In the most frequently observed category, the number of fruits served per day was 0–5 for all countries. The majority of respondents in China and South Africa consumed between 6–10 vegetables per day, while their counterparts in Ghana, India, and Russia averaged between 0–5 vegetables served per day. The most frequently observed category of self-reported memory difficulty was moderate for China, Ghana, India, and Russia, while South Africa was Good (n = 1194, 48%). Supplementary material Table A2 shows the summary statistics for age, education, cognitive functioning and income (please see supplementary material Table A2).

Meta-regression analysis

Figure 1 shows a forest plot of the impact of SES on cognitive functioning in the selected countries. The meta-regression analysis revealed a pooled cognitive functioning of 2% (95% CI: 0.02–0.03, I2 = 93.4%, p < 0.001). This indicates that on average, a one-unit increase in the SES will increase the value of cognitive functioning by 0.02, giving credence to the fact that there is strong evidence that SES does improve cognitive functioning. Similar trend was also observed for lifestyle (0.02 (95% CI: 0.01, 0.04, I2 = 89.1%, p < 0.001). A pooled cognitive functioning of -0.01 (95% CI: -0.02,-0.01, I2 = 92.0%, p < 0.001), -0.03 (95% CI: -0.04,-0.02, I2 = 95.2%, p < 0.001), -0.00 (95% CI: -0.01, 0.01, I2 = 84.6%, p < 0.001) was observed for demographic characteristics, self-reported memory difficulty and chronic diseases, respectively (please see supplementary material Figure A1, A2, A3 and A4).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Forest plot of the impact of socioeconomic status on cognitive functioning among the five selected countries

Hierarchical linear regression

A five-step hierarchical linear regression was conducted with cognitive functioning as the dependent variable. The hierarchical regression analysis results consist of model comparisons and a model interpretation based on an alpha of 0.05. Each step in the hierarchical regression was compared to the previous step using F-tests.

Model comparison

The F-test for Step 1 was significant for China, Ghana, India, Russia, and South Africa, indicating that adding sex, age, residence, and marital status explained an additional 6.92%, 2.99%, 1.47%, 9.86%, and 2.32%, respectively, of the variation in cognitive functioning in the five selected countries. The F-test for step 2 was significant for China, Ghana, Russia, and South Africa, but not India. This suggests that adding self-reported memory difficulty explained an additional 0.91%, 0.69%, 0.87%, and 2.26% of the variation in cognitive functioning in China, Ghana, Russia, and South Africa, respectively. Adding tobacco use, alcohol use, vegetable and fruit servings per day yielded a significant F-test for step 3 for China, Ghana, Russia, and South Africa; however, this was not the case for India. Furthermore, introducing chronic diseases in step 4 produced significant F-test for China, Ghana, and Russia, but not for India and South Africa. Finally, the F-test for step 5 was significant for all five countries, indicating that adding education and income explained an additional 2.49%, 0.49%, 1.19%, 2.17%, and 2.50% of the variation in cognitive functioning for China, Ghana, India, Russia, and South Africa, respectively, as shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Model comparisons for variables predicting cognitive functioning

Model interpretation

Sex (B = -0.14, t (2468) = -2.47, p = 0.014.), age (B = -0.06, t (2468) = -4.94, p < 0.001), residence (B = -0.10, t (2468) = -3.78, p < 0.001) and self-reported memory difficulty (B = -0.06, t (2468) = -4.10, p < 0.001), significantly predicted cognitive functioning in China. These indicate that on average, a differences in sex, a one-unit increase of age, a change in the type of residence and a one-unit increase of self-reported memory difficulty will decrease the value of cognitive functioning by 0.14, 0.06, 0.10 and 0.06 units respectively. Also, vegetables per day (B = 0.06, t (2468) = 4.55, p < 0.001), anxiety (B = 0.05, t (2468) = 2.24, p = 0.025), stroke (B = 0.14, t (2468) = 2.15, p = 0.032), education (B = 0.05, t (2468) = 5.06, p < 0.001), and income (B = 0.05, t (2468) = 5.61, p < 0.001) significantly predicted cognitive functioning in China. These indicate that on average, a one-unit increase in vegetable consumption per day, anxiety level, stroke, education, and income will increase the value of cognitive functioning by 0.05, 0.06, 0.05, 0.14 and 0.05 units respectively. However, marital status, tobacco use, alcohol use, fruits per day, chronic lung disease, hypertension, diabetes and depression did not significantly predict cognitive functioning, suggesting that, a one-unit increase or change in any of these variables do not have significant effect on cognitive functioning in China.

For Ghana, age (B = -0.03, t (2468) = -2.74, p = 0.006), self-reported memory difficulty (B = -0.06, t (2468) = -3.42, p < 0.001) and anxiety (B = -0.10, t (2468) = -5.89, p < 0.001), significantly predicted cognitive functioning. These indicate that on average, a one-unit increase of age, self-reported memory difficulty and anxiety will decrease the value of cognitive functioning by 0.03, 0.06 and 0.10 units respectively. Also, residence (B = 0.10, t (2468) = 3.70, p < 0.001, alcohol (B = 0.08, t (2468) = 3.08, p = 0.002), fruits per day (B = 0.21, t (2468) = 3.78, p < 0.001), anxiety (B = -0.10, t (2468) = -5.89, p < 0.001), stroke (B = 0.23, t (2468) = 3.14, p = 0.002)), education (B = 0.02, t (2468) = 1.98, p = 0.048) and income (B = 0.02, t (2468) = 2.49, p = 0.013) significantly predicted cognitive functioning in Ghana. These indicate that on average, a change in residence, a one-unit increase in the level of education, alcohol use, fruits per day, anxiety, stroke and income will increase the value of cognitive functioning by 0.10, 0.02, 0.08, 0.21, 0.10, 0.23 and 0.02 units respectively. However, sex, marital status, tobacco use, vegetables per day, chronic lung disease, hypertension, diabetes and depression did not significantly predict cognitive functioning. Based on these samples, a one-unit increase or change in any of these variables do not have significant effect on cognitive functioning in Ghana.

Sex (B = -0.21, t (2469) = -2.89, p = 0.004) and marital status (B = -0.01, t (2469) = -2.81, p = 0.005) significantly predicted cognitive functioning in India. These indicate that, on average, a change in marital status and difference in sex will decrease the value of cognitive functioning by 0.21 and 0.10 units. Also, residence (B = 0.03, t (2469) = 2.07, p = 0.038), education (B = 0.06, t(2469) = 2.63, p = 0.009) and income (B = 0.02, t (2468) = 2.49, p = 0.013) significantly predicted cognitive functioning, indicating that on average, a change in the type of residence, educational level and a one-unit increase of income will increase the value of cognitive functioning by 0.03, 0.06 and 0.02 units respectively in India. However, age, self-reported memory difficulty, alcohol use, vegetables per day, fruits per day, chronic lung disease, hypertension, diabetes, anxiety, stroke, and depression did not significantly predict cognitive functioning.

For Russia, age (B = -0.09, t (2468) = -7.25, p < 0.00), self-reported memory difficulty (B = -0.07, t (2468) = -3.04, p = 0.002), alcohol use (B = -0.06, t (2468) = -2.02, p = 0.043), tobacco use (B = -0.08, t (2468) = -2.19, p = 0.029), and residence (B = -0.12, t (2468) = -3.90, p < 0.001) significantly predicted cognitive functioning. These indicate that on average, a one-unit increase of age, self-reported memory difficulty, alcohol use, tobacco use, and a change in residence, will decrease the value of cognitive functioning by 0.09, 0.12, 0.07, 0.06 and 0.08 units respectively. Also, sex (B = 0.09, t (2468) = 2.56, p = 0.011), stroke (B = 0.14, t (2468) = 2.20, p = 0.028), depression (B = 0.16, t (2468) = 2.30, p = 0.021, education (B = 0.11, t (2468) = 6.92, p < 0.001), and income (B = 0.03, t (2468) = 2.83, p = 0.005) significantly predicted cognitive functioning in Russia. These indicate that on average, a change in the category of sex, a one-unit increase in stroke, depression, level of education and income will increase the value of cognitive functioning by 0.09, 0.14, 0.16, 0.11 and 0.03 units respectively. However, marital status, fruits per day, vegetables per day, chronic lung disease, hypertension, diabetes, anxiety and depression did not significantly predict cognitive functioning, suggesting that, a one-unit increase or change in any of these variables do not have significant effect on cognitive functioning.

Age (B = -0.04, t (2468) = -4.04, p < 0.001) and self-reported memory difficulty (B = -0.09, t (2468) = -5.92, p < 0.001), significantly predicted cognitive functioning in South Africa. These indicate that on average, a one-unit increase in age and self-reported memory difficulty will decrease the value of cognitive functioning by 0.04 and 0.09 units respectively. Further, residence (B = 0.11, t (2468) = 4.70, p < 0.001), vegetables per day (B = 0.27, t (2468) = 4.35, p < 0.001), education (B = 0.02, t (2468) = 2.28, p = 0.023) and income (B = 0.06, t (2468) = 6.44, p < 0.001) significantly predicted cognitive functioning, indicating that on average, a change in residence, a one-unit increase in vegetable consumption per day, educational level, and income will increase the value of cognitive functioning by 0.11, 0.27, 0.02, and 0.06 units respectively in South Africa. However, sex, marital status, alcohol use, tobacco use, fruits per day, chronic lung disease, hypertension, diabetes, anxiety, stroke and depression did not significantly predict cognitive functioning. Based on these samples, a one-unit increase in any of these variables do not have significant effect on cognitive functioning. The result for each regression is shown in Table 3.

Table 3 Summary of hierarchical regression analysis for variables predicting cognitive functioning in the five selected countries

Discussion

This study examined the impact of socioeconomic status (assessed using income and education), demographics, risk factors such as lifestyle, chronic diseases and self-rated memory difficulty on adults' cognitive functioning in five selected countries via a comparative analysis of a population-based sample from the Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health Wave 1. This study found income to significantly predict cognitive functioning in all five selected countries. This indicates that, on average, a one-unit increase in one's income will increase an adult's cognitive functioning by 0.05, 0.02, 0.09 0.03, and 0.06 units for China, Ghana, India, Russia, and South Africa, respectively. This is consistent with previous studies on the role of income on the cognitive functioning of individuals [58,59,60]. The socioeconomic status tends to influence the development of brain structure in childhood [61], and whether or not the brain structure is formed properly as a child grows into adulthood (hippocampal formation) and even into old age could have dire consequences such as an increased risk of Alzheimer's' Disease in later life [62]. Low-income adults rarely engage in healthy lifestyles, mentally stimulating activities and may have restricted access to resources and adequate healthcare, like visiting a neuropsychologist because of low or no disposable income; this could increase the probability of cognitive decline.

The study also found education to significantly predict cognitive functioning in all the selected countries, suggesting that a one-unit increase in an individual's level of education will increase their cognitive functioning. This observation is consistent with a study by Wu et al., 2011, who found an association between a low level of education and a high risk of cognitive impairment among older adults in Taiwan [63]. Similar studies in Ghana [64], Russia [65] and South Africa [66] have noted an association between education and cognitive functioning. Research has shown that highly educated individuals tend to have greater brain reserve capacity than those with no formal education [67]. It is also believed that highly educated people are more likely to seek emotional support which can consequently result in a positive change in the function and structure of their brain [64, 68] as opposed to people with no or low level of education.

Our study found that age significantly predicted cognitive functioning in China, Ghana, Russia, and South Africa. This finding is consistent with previous studies, which suggested that age is a risk factor in cognitive decline among adults [23,24,25]. A study by Murman, 2015 revealed that the aging of adults tends to accelerate age-related diseases such as dementia by increasing the rate of neuronal dysfunction, neuronal loss, and cognitive decline [29]. These age-related risks also have the propensity to impair the everyday functional abilities of adults, especially in the context of developing countries. Furthermore, the type of residency (rural or urban) significantly predicted cognitive functioning in all the selected countries. A study by Yuan et al. 2020 observed that individuals residing in an urban area have better cognition [36] due to perceived better income conditions than their counterparts in rural residences. This study observed that most respondents in China, Ghana, and India lived in rural areas. Rural communities are plagued with lack of or low access to healthcare, high cost of care, a higher proportion of expenditure to income, lack of sustainable employment, and lack of access to basic social amenities. These issues tend to make rural-dwellers more susceptible to diseases. Hence, there is the need to design policies such as rolling out tailor-made insurance products, deliberate allocation of funds targeted at improving access to healthcare, and reducing the risks associated with living in these rural areas. These measures have the propensity to enhance the cognitive functioning of adults who live in rural areas. Additionally stakeholders must continue to create ecosystems that will allow individuals to venture into private businesses. They must also improve access to gainful middle-level employment opportunities for graduate adults [69], as studies have found that urban adults dwellers who are satisfied with their income conditions have improved cognitive functioning [36]. It is also essential to reduce the high cost associated with healthcare, increase access to healthcare delivery, and improve transportation systems in urban areas. It is also necessary to create avenues for exercise [70], leisure, recreation, and relaxation [71], to reduce the burden associated with urban living, as these will eventually lead to improved cognitive functioning, particularly in Russia and South Africa where the majority of respondents were urban dwellers.

Sex was observed to significantly predict cognitive functioning in China, India and Russia but not in Ghana and South Africa. This study's observed difference in cognitive functioning between males and females may be explained from two angles. First, females have a longer life expectancy than males and are likely to experience more aging-related cognitive impairments [72, 73]. Second, females in most developing nations are generally disadvantaged socially and economically compared to their male counterparts; hence they lack the needed resources to access decent healthcare and have favorable health outcomes, culminating in cognitive limitation [74].

This study observed that self-reported memory difficulty significantly predicted cognitive functioning in all the selected countries; China, Ghana, Russia, and South Africa, except India. This suggests that, on average, a one-unit increase in an individual's self-reported memory difficulty will decrease the value of cognitive functioning by 0.06 units for China and Ghana, 0.07 units for Russia, and 0.09 units for South Africa. Previous studies have shown the effect of memory difficulty on cognitive functioning [75,76,77,78]. As people age, they experience significant deficits in their memories. These deficits are primarily manifested in daily activities such as decision-making, problem-solving, and the planning of goal-directed behaviors coordinated by active manipulation, reorganization, and integrating the contents of one's memory [75]. These activities are relevant for the effective and efficient performance of these higher-level cognitive functions [75, 78]. Hence, individuals need to continue to include physical activities into their daily routines, socialize regularly, and manage stress levels to reduce the burden on their memories and consequently pre-empt cognitive decline.

Regarding the impact of lifestyle choices on adults' cognitive functioning, this study found tobacco use to significantly predict cognitive functioning in only Russia . Previous studies have revealed that people who smoke are at a higher risk of developing aging-related cognitive diseases such as all types of dementia and are at an even higher risk for Alzheimer's disease [79]. Though this study did not find tobacco use to significantly predict cognitive functioning in China, Ghana, India, and South Africa, there is the need to explore further the effect of tobacco use in these four countries. This study suspects that this disparity could be accounted for by the possible smoking status of respondents, that is, whether a smoker is currently smoking or not. The status of the smoker is pertinent because studies have shown that current smokers are at higher risk [80, 81] and experience a faster cognitive decline [82] than former smokers [83]. The study also revealed that alcohol use significantly predicted cognitive functioning in Ghana and Russia but not in China, India, and South Africa.

Fruits consumed per day were significant only in Ghana. In contrast, vegetable consumption per day was significant in predicting cognitive functioning in China and South Africa. This finding is consistent with previous studies by Chen et al. 2012 [59], who found that lower intakes of vegetables are associated with cognitive decline among elderly Chinese. Several laboratories [84, 85] and epidemiological studies [86,87,88,89] have shown that antioxidants, which are found in fruits and vegetables, are associated with cognitive function [59]. Fruits and vegetables contain vitamins C, E, folate, and carotenoids, which have been found to improve cognitive functions among higher consumers of these nutrients. Hence, individuals must introduce the habit of eating fruits and vegetables, if they wish to improve their cognitive functioning.

Finally, in terms of chronic diseases, this study did not find a significant effect of diabetes, hypertension, and chronic lung disease on cognitive functioning in all countries; meanwhile, anxiety was found to predict cognitive functioning in China and Ghana significantly. Stroke was found to significantly predict cognitive functioning in China, Ghana, and Russia, which is consistent with previous studies that have shown an association between stroke and cognitive functioning [90].

Clinical significance and implications

Though this study found SES, some demographic characteristics, and risk factors to significantly  predict cognitive functioning, the magnitude of the effect is relatively small. For instance, the effect of SES and lifestyle on cognitive functioning was 2%, suggesting that SES and lifestyle only explained a 0.02 variability to cognitive functioning. Thus, it may appear farfetched to claim that socioeconomic status and risk factors clinically influence cognitive functioning. That notwithstanding, it should be noted that this study evaluated a large number of potential explanatory variables and that some factors (medical variables such as chronic diseases) explained even lower variance to cognitive functioning and were mostly not significant. When interpreting test results in clinical practice, neuropsychologists examine several medical and demographical factors [91, 92]. The purpose of this study was to provide insights into social factors that could influence cognitive deficiencies, so clinicians will not limit their investigation to only neurological and pharmacological considerations when assessing cognitive functioning. As a result, improving social situations may help in improving cognitive functioning. Our findings underscore the need for an interdisciplinary approach towards addressing cognitive limitations among doctors, psychologists, and social workers, ensuring more holistic healthcare to citizens of these countries.

Conclusion

The results of this study highlight the predicting factors of cognitive functioning across all ages in developing countries. The study further analyzed the impact of SES, lifestyle, demographic characteristics and other risk factors on cognitive functioning in the context of developing countries. Based on the study findings, stakeholdersmust implement policies, encourage participation in daily physical activities, and invest in cheaper and accessible healthcare. It is also imperative to improve school enrolments for younger and middle-aged adults while strengthening adult education initiatives. These measures will help eliminate the risk factors associated with cognitive decline and improve the socioeconomic status of cohorts.

Availability of data and materials

The data used for this study is the Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health (SAGE) Wave 1 conducted by the World Health Organization in six participating countries. These data are publicly available on the National Archive of Computerized Data on Aging (NACDA) website https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/web/ICPSR/studies/31381/datadocumentation.

References

  1. You D, Anthony D. Generation 2025 and beyond: The critical importance of understanding demographic trends for children of the 21st century ©UNICEF, Division of Policy and Strategy; 2012.

  2. Larnyo E, Dai B, Akey-Torku B, Nutakor J, Tetteh E, Larnyo A, et al. Evaluating Ghanaian Family Carers' Perceptions on the Use of Healthcare Wearable Devices by Dementia Patients. International Journal of Scientific Research in Science, Engineering and Technology(IJSRSET). 2020;7(2):612–27.DOI: doi.org/https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.32628/IJSRSET2072117

  3. United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs, Population Division (2015). Population 2030: Demographic challenges and opportunities for sustainable development planning (ST/ESA/SER.A/389).

  4. Dai B, Larnyo E, Tetteh EA, Aboagye AK, Musah A-AI. Factors Affecting Caregivers’ Acceptance of the Use of Wearable Devices by Patients With Dementia: An Extension of the Unified Theory of Acceptance and Use of Technology Model. American Journal of Alzheimer's Disease & Other Dementias. 2020;35:1533317519883493

  5. Salthouse TA. Why Are There Different Age Relations in Cross-Sectional and Longitudinal Comparisons of Cognitive Functioning? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2014;23(4):252–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/0963721414535212.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Batty GD, Deary IJ, Zaninotto P. Association of Cognitive Function With Cause-Specific Mortality in Middle and Older Age: Follow-up of Participants in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Am J Epidemiol. 2016;183(3):183–90. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/aje/kwv139.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Gottfredson LS, Deary IJ. Intelligence Predicts Health and Longevity, but Why? Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2004;13(1):1–4. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301001.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. McGuire LC, Ford ES, Ajani UA. The impact of cognitive functioning on mortality and the development of functional disability in older adults with diabetes: the second longitudinal study on aging. BMC Geriatr. 2006;6:8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2318-6-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Singh P, Govil D, Kumar V, Kumar J. Cognitive Impairment and Quality of Life among Elderly in India. Appl Res Qual Life. 2017;12(4):963–79. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s11482-016-9499-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Ramírez-Vélez R, Correa-Bautista JE. Reference values for handgrip strength and their association with intrinsic capacity domains among older adults. 2019;10(2):278–86. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/jcsm.12373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Wu F, Guo Y, Zheng Y, Ma W, Kowal P, Chatterji S, et al. Social-Economic Status and Cognitive Performance among Chinese Aged 50 Years and Older. PLoS ONE. 2016;11: e0166986. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1371/journal.pone.0166986.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Yang L, Martikainen P, Silventoinen K, Konttinen H. Association of socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning change among elderly Chinese people. Age Ageing. 2016;45(5):674–80. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/ageing/afw107.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Skirbekk V, Loichinger E, Weber D. Variation in cognitive functioning as a refined approach to comparing aging across countries. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2012;109(3):770. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1073/pnas.1112173109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Fors S, Lennartsson C, Lundberg O. Childhood living conditions, socioeconomic position in adulthood, and cognition in later life: exploring the associations. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2009;64(6):750–7. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/geronb/gbp029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Selvamani Y, Arokiasamy P. Association of life course socioeconomic status and adult height with cognitive functioning of older adults in India and China. BMC Geriatr. 2021;21(1):354. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12877-021-02303-w.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Peltzer K, Phaswana-Mafuya N. Cognitive functioning and associated factors in older adults in South Africa. S Afr J Psychiatry. 2012;18:157–62.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Miu J, Negin J, Salinas-Rodriguez A, Manrique-Espinoza B, Sosa-Ortiz AL, Cumming R, et al. Factors associated with cognitive function in older adults in Mexico. Glob Health Action. 2016;9:30747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Susilowati IH, Yasukouchi A. Cognitive characteristics of older Japanese drivers. J Physiol Anthropol. 2012;31:1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Van Hooren SAH, Valentijn AM, Bosma H, Ponds R, Van Boxtel MPJ, Jolles J. Cognitive functioning in healthy older adults aged 64–81: a cohort study into the effects of age, sex, and education. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2007;14:40–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Maharani A, Tampubolon G. National economic development status may affect the association between central adiposity and cognition in older adults. PloS one. 2016;11:e0148406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Ramachandran V, Aryani P. Association between educational level and hypertension with decrease of cognitive function among elderly at Puskesmas Mengwi I. Intisari Sains Medis. 2018;9:43–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Yount KM, Hoddinott J, Stein AD, DiGirolamo AM. Individual capital and cognitive ageing in Guatemala. Popul Stud. 2009;63:295–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Maharani A. Childhood socioeconomic status and cognitive function later in life: evidence from a national survey in Indonesia. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. 2020;33(4):214–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Kumar H, Arokiasamy P, Selvamani Y. Socioeconomic disadvantage, chronic diseases and their association with cognitive functioning of adults in india: A multilevel analysis. Journal of Population Ageing. 2020;13(3):285–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Selvamani Y, Singh P. Socioeconomic patterns of underweight and its association with self-rated health, cognition and quality of life among older adults in India. PLoS One. 2018;13(3):e0193979.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. Schafer MH, Shippee TP. Age Identity, Gender, and Perceptions of Decline: Does Feeling Older Lead to Pessimistic Dispositions About Cognitive Aging? The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2009;65B(1):91–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/geronb/gbp046%JTheJournalsofGerontology:SeriesB.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Stephan Y, Caudroit J, Chalabaev A. Subjective health and memory self-efficacy as mediators in the relation between subjective age and life satisfaction among older adults. Aging Ment Health. 2011;15(4):428–36. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/13607863.2010.536138.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Valentijn SA, Hill RD, Van Hooren SA, Bosma H, Van Boxtel MP, Jolles J, et al. Memory self-efficacy predicts memory performance: results from a 6-year follow-up study. Jounral of Psychology Aging & Mental Health. 2006;21(1):165.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Murman DL. The Impact of Age on Cognition. Seminars in Hearing. 2015;36(3):111–21. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1055/s-0035-1555115.

  30. Sbarra DA. Marriage protects men from clinically meaningful elevations in C-reactive protein: results from the National Social Life, Health, and Aging Project (NSHAP). Psychosom Med. 2009;71(8):828–35. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1097/psy.0b013e3181b4c4f2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Liu H, Waite L. Bad marriage, broken heart? Age and gender differences in the link between marital quality and cardiovascular risks among older adults. J Health Soc Behav. 2014;55(4):403–23. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/0022146514556893.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Liu H, Umberson DJ. The Times They Are a Changin’: Marital Status and Health Differentials from 1972 to 2003. J Health Soc Behav. 2008;49(3):239–53. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1177/002214650804900301.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. Liu H, Zhang Y, Burgard SA, Needham BL. Marital status and cognitive impairment in the United States: evidence from the National Health and Aging Trends Study. Ann Epidemiol. 2019;38:28-34.e2. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.annepidem.2019.08.007.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Feng L, Ng XT, Yap P, Li J, Lee TS, Håkansson K, et al. Marital Status and Cognitive Impairment among Community-Dwelling Chinese Older Adults: The Role of Gender and Social Engagement. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders Extra. 2014;4(3):375–84. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1159/000358584.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Sundström A, Westerlund O, Kotyrlo E. Marital status and risk of dementia: a nationwide population-based prospective study from Sweden. BMJ Open. 2016;6(1): e008565. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-008565.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Yuan M, Fu H, Han Y, Chen J, Fang Y. Mediation and moderated mediation in the relationships among income condition, depression, cognition and residence among older adults in China. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2020;20(10):860–6. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/ggi.13987.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Nakamura K, Kitamura K, Watanabe Y, Shinoda H, Sato H, Someya T. Rural–urban differences in the prevalence of cognitive impairment in independent community-dwelling elderly residents of Ojiya city, Niigata Prefecture. Japan Environmental Health and Preventive Medicine. 2016;21(6):422–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s12199-016-0542-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Gur RC, Mozley PD, Resnick SM, Gottlieb GL, Kohn M, Zimmerman R, et al. Gender differences in age effect on brain atrophy measured by magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1991;88(7):2845–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1073/pnas.88.7.2845.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Aartsen MJ, Martin M, Zimprich D. Gender Differences in Level and Change in Cognitive Functioning. Gerontology. 2004;50(1):35–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1159/000074387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Lee J, Shih R, Feeney K, Langa KM. Gender Disparity in Late-life Cognitive Functioning in India: Findings From the Longitudinal Aging Study in India. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B. 2014;69(4):603–11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1093/geronb/gbu017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Remes O, Wainwright N, Surtees P, Lafortune L, Khaw K-T, Brayne C. Sex differences in the association between area deprivation and generalised anxiety disorder: British population study. BMJ Open. 2017;7(5): e013590. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmjopen-2016-013590.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  42. Upadhayay N, Guragain S. Comparison of cognitive functions between male and female medical students: a pilot study. Journal of clinical and diagnostic research : JCDR. 2014;8(6):Bc12–5.DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.7860/jcdr/2014/7490.4449.

  43. Finkel D, Reynolds CA, McArdle JJ, Gatz M, Pedersen NL. Latent growth curve analyses of accelerating decline in cognitive abilities in late adulthood. Dev Psychol. 2003;39(3):535–50. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.535.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Richards M, Shipley B, Fuhrer R, Wadsworth MEJ. Cognitive ability in childhood and cognitive decline in mid-life: longitudinal birth cohort study. BMJ. 2004;328:552.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Nutakor JA, Dai B, Gavu AK, Antwi O-A. Relationship between chronic diseases and sleep duration among older adults in Ghana. Quality of Life Research. 2020:1–10.

  46. Nutakor JA, Dai B, Zhou J, Larnyo E, Gavu AK, Asare MK. Association between socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning among older adults in Ghana. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2020:1–10.DOI: doi.org/https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1002/gps.5475.

  47. Fernández-Niño JA, Bonilla-Tinoco LJ, Manrique-Espinoza BS, Romero-Martínez M, Sosa-Ortiz AL. Work status, retirement, and depression in older adults: An analysis of six countries based on the Study on Global Ageing and Adult Health (SAGE). SSM - Population Health. 2018;6:1–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.ssmph.2018.07.008.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Chatterji S, Kowal P. WHO Study on Global AGEing and Adult Health (SAGE): Wave 1, 2007–2010 Individual Respondent Level Codebook*. Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106 Inter-university Consortium for Political and Social Research; 2010.

  49. Cagney KA, Lauderdale DS. Education, wealth, and cognitive function in later life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57:P163–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Evans DA, Beckett LA, Albert MS, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, Funkenstein HH, et al. Level of education and change in cognitive function in a community population of older persons. Ann Epidemiol. 1993;3:71–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Kubzansky LD, Berkman LF, Glass TA, Seeman TE. Is educational attainment associated with shared determinants of health in the elderly? Findings from the MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Psychosom Med. 1998;60:578–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  52. Koster A, Penninx BWJH, Bosma H, Kempen GIJM, Newman AB, Rubin SM, et al. Socioeconomic differences in cognitive decline and the role of biomedical factors. Ann Epidemiol. 2005;15:564–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Lee S, Kawachi I, Berkman LF, Grodstein F. Education, other socioeconomic indicators, and cognitive function. Am J Epidemiol. 2003;157:712–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Westfall P, Henning K. Texts in statistical science: Understanding advanced statistical methods. Boca Raton, FL: Taylor & Francis; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Higgins JP, Thompson SG, Deeks JJ, Altman DG. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. BMJ Open. 2003;327(7414):557–60. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Dall’Oglio AM, Rossiello B, Coletti MF, Caselli MC, Ravà L, di Ciommo V, et al. Developmental evaluation at age 4: Validity of an Italian parental questionnaire. J Paediatr Child Health. 2010;46(7–8):419–26. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1111/j.1440-1754.2010.01748.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Hinton P, McMurray I, Brownlow C. SPSS Explained. 2nd ed: Routledge; 2014.DOI: doi.org/https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4324/9781315797298.

  58. Bajaj JS, Riggio O, Allampati S, Prakash R, Gioia S, Onori E, et al. Cognitive dysfunction is associated with poor socioeconomic status in patients with cirrhosis: an international multicenter study. Clin Gastroenterol Hepatol. 2013;11(11):1511–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Chen X, Huang Y, Cheng H. Lower intake of vegetables and legumes associated with cognitive decline among illiterate elderly Chinese: a 3-year cohort study. J Nutr Health Aging. 2012;16(6):549–52.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  60. Xu X, Liang J, Bennett JM, Botoseneanu A, Allore HG. Socioeconomic stratification and multidimensional health trajectories: evidence of convergence in later old age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;70(4):661–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Maggi S, Irwin LG, Siddiqi A, Poureslami I, Hertzman E, Hertzman C. Analytic and strategic review paper: International perspectives on early child development. eSocialSciences Working Papers. 2006(id: 690).

  62. Moceri VM, Kukull WA, Emanual I, van Belle G, Starr JR, Schellenberg GD, et al. Using census data and birth certificates to reconstruct the early-life socioeconomic environment and the relation to the development of Alzheimer's disease. Epidemiology. 2001:383–9.

  63. Wu M-S, Lan T-H, Chen C-M, Chiu H-C, Lan T-Y. Socio-demographic and health-related factors associated with cognitive impairment in the elderly in Taiwan. BMC Public Health. 2011;11(1):1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Nutakor JA, Dai B, Zhou J, Larnyo E, Gavu AK, Asare MK. Association between socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning among older adults in Ghana. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry. 2020.

  65. Grigorenko EL. Current trends in education in Russia: Preliminary outcomes indicative of students’ cognitive development. Int J Psychol. 1999;34(3):175–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Peltzer K, Phaswana-Mafuya N. Cognitive functioning and associated factors in older adults in South Africa. S Afr J Psychiatry. 2012;18(4):157–62.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Schmand B, Smit JH, Geerlings MI, Lindeboom J. The effects of intelligence and education on the development of dementia. A test of the brain reserve hypothesis. Psychological medicine. 1997;27(6):1337–44.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Beydoun MA, Beydoun HA, Gamaldo AA, Teel A, Zonderman AB, Wang Y. Epidemiologic studies of modifiable factors associated with cognition and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Vance DE, Bail J, Enah CC, Palmer JJ, Hoenig AK. The impact of employment on cognition and cognitive reserve: implications across diseases and aging. Nursing: Research and Reviews. 2016;6:61–71.

    Google Scholar 

  70. Siu M-y, Lee DTF. Effects of tai chi on cognition and instrumental activities of daily living in community dwelling older people with mild cognitive impairment. BMC Geriatrics. 2018;18(1):37.DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12877-018-0720-8.

  71. Duvivier BMFM, Schaper NC, Koster A, van Kan L, Peters HPF, Adam JJ, et al. Benefits of Substituting Sitting with Standing and Walking in Free-Living Conditions for Cardiometabolic Risk Markers, Cognition and Mood in Overweight Adults. Frontiers in Physiology. 2017;8(353).DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.3389/fphys.2017.00353.

  72. Murtagh KN, Hubert HB. Gender Differences in Physical Disability Among an Elderly Cohort. 2004;94(8):1406–11. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.2105/ajph.94.8.1406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. von Strauss E, Agüero-Torres H, Kåreholt I, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L. Women are more disabled in basic activities of daily living than men only in very advanced ages: a study on disability, morbidity, and mortality from the Kungsholmen Project. J Clin Epidemiol. 2003;56(7):669–77. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/s0895-4356(03)00089-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Amegbor PM, Kuuire VZ, Robertson H, Kuffuor OA. Predictors of basic self-care and intermediate self-care functional disabilities among older adults in Ghana. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2018;77:81–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.archger.2018.04.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Glisky EL. Changes in Cognitive Function in Human Aging. In: Riddle DR, editor. Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. Boca Raton (FL): CRC Press/Taylor & Francis; 2007. Chapter 1. PMID: 21204355.

  76. Reuter-Lorenz PA, Sylvester C-YC. The cognitive neuroscience of working memory and aging. 2005.

  77. Zacks R, Hasher L, Li K. Human memory In Craik FIM & Salthouse TA (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (pp. 293–357). Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers[Google Scholar]. 2000.

  78. Naveh-Benjamin, M., Moscovitch, M., & Roediger, III, H.L. (Eds.). (2002). Perspectives on Human Memory and Cognitive Aging: Essays in Honor of Fergus Craik (1st ed.). Psychology Press. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.4324/9780203759707

  79. Barnes DE, Yaffe K. The projected effect of risk factor reduction on Alzheimer’s disease prevalence. The Lancet Neurology. 2011;10(9):819–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Durazzo TC, Mattsson N, Weiner MW; Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative. Smoking and increased Alzheimer's disease risk: a review of potential mechanisms. Alzheimers Dement. 2014 Jun;10(3 Suppl):S122-45. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.04.009. PMID: 24924665; PMCID: PMC4098701.

  81. Mons U, Schöttker B, Müller H, Kliegel M, Brenner H. History of lifetime smoking, smoking cessation and cognitive function in the elderly population. Eur J Epidemiol. 2013;28(10):823–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Sabia S, Elbaz A, Dugravot A, Head J, Shipley M, Hagger-Johnson G, et al. Impact of smoking on cognitive decline in early old age: the Whitehall II cohort study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(6):627–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Peters R, Poulter R, Warner J, Beckett N, Burch L, Bulpitt C. Smoking, dementia and cognitive decline in the elderly, a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2008;8(1):36. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1471-2318-8-36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Cotman CW, Head E, Muggenburg BA, Zicker S, Milgram NW. Brain aging in the canine: a diet enriched in antioxidants reduces cognitive dysfunction. Neurobiol Aging. 2002;23(5):809–18.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  85. Kruman II, Kumaravel T, Lohani A, Pedersen WA, Cutler RG, Kruman Y, et al. Folic acid deficiency and homocysteine impair DNA repair in hippocampal neurons and sensitize them to amyloid toxicity in experimental models of Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci. 2002;22(5):1752–62.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  86. Velho S, Marques-Vidal P, Baptista F, Camilo ME. Dietary intake adequacy and cognitive function in free-living active elderly: a cross-sectional and short-term prospective study. Clin Nutr. 2008;27(1):77–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Psaltopoulou T, Kyrozis A, Stathopoulos P, Trichopoulos D, Vassilopoulos D, Trichopoulou A. Diet, physical activity and cognitive impairment among elders: the EPIC–Greece cohort (European Prospective Investigation into Cancer and Nutrition). Public Health Nutr. 2008;11(10):1054–62.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  88. Gustaw-Rothenberg K. Dietary patterns associated with Alzheimer’s disease: population based study. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2009;6(4):1335–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Gu Y, Nieves JW, Stern Y, Luchsinger JA, Scarmeas N. Food combination and Alzheimer disease risk: a protective diet. Arch Neurol. 2010;67(6):699–706.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  90. Kalaria RN, Ballard C. Stroke and cognition. Curr Atheroscler Rep. 2001;3(4):334–9. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/s11883-001-0028-5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. Hohmann L, Holtkamp M, Oltmanns F, Bengner T. Associations of individual and structural socioeconomic status with cognition and mental distress in pharmacoresistant focal epilepsy. Epilepsy Behav. 2021;116:107726. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.yebeh.2020.107726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Kanner AM, Helmstaedter C, Sadat-Hossieny Z, Meador K. Cognitive disorders in epilepsy I: Clinical experience, real-world evidence and recommendations. Seizure. 2020;83:216–22. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.seizure.2020.10.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to The National Archive of Computerized Data on Aging (NACDA) for granting the researchers access to the data. The authors would like to sincerely thank the editor and anonymous reviewers for their insightful comments and suggestions resulting in a much-improved version of this manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded by the National Nature Science Foundation of China (71774069).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, E. L. and B. D.; Formal analysis, E. L. and J. A. N; Funding acquisition, B. D.; Investigation, E. L.; Methodology, E. L. and J. A. N.; Resources, A. L. and R.A.; Software, E. L.; Supervision, B. D.; Validation, E. L. and A. L.; Writing – original draft, E. L., J. A. N., S.A.W, A. L., and R.A.; Writing – review & editing, E. L., B. D., J. A. N., S.A.W, A. L., and R.A. The authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Baozhen Dai.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The procedures untaken in this study and the data collection processes were conducted ethically per the World Medical Association's Declaration of Helsinki. Ethical approvals were obtained for SAGE study from the Ethics Review Committee of the World Health Organization, the Ethics and Protocol Review Committee of the Ghana Medical School, Accra, Ghana, the Ethics Committee of the School of Preventive and Social Medicine, and the Russian Academy of Medical Sciences, Moscow, Russia. Approval was also obtained for the SAGE 1 study from the Ethics Committee of the Shanghai Municipal Centre for Disease Control and Prevention, Shanghai, China, Institutional Review Board of the International Institute of Population Sciences, Mumbai, India, and finally from the Research Ethics Committee of the Human Sciences Research Council, Pretoria, South Africa. These approvals also covered all procedures through which written informed consent was obtained from each participant. Confidential records of participants' consent were maintained by SAGE. Further, written informed consents for participating in the SAGE 1 study were obtained from each participant.

Consent for publication

All authors consent to the publication of this research.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Larnyo, E., Dai, B., Nutakor, J.A. et al. Examining the impact of socioeconomic status, demographic characteristics, lifestyle and other risk factors on adults' cognitive functioning in developing countries: an analysis of five selected WHO SAGE Wave 1 Countries. Int J Equity Health 21, 31 (2022). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12939-022-01622-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12939-022-01622-7

Keywords